Contemporary politics,local and international current affairs, science, music and extracts from the Queensland Newspaper "THE WORKER" documenting the proud history of the Labour Movement.
MAHATMA GANDHI ~ Truth never damages a cause that is just.
Wednesday, 29 March 2017
Alien intelligence: the extraordinary minds of octopuses and other cephalopods
After a startling encounter with a cuttlefish, Australian philosopher
Peter Godfrey-Smith set out to explore the mysterious lives of
cephalopods. He was left asking: why do such smart, optimistic creatures
live such a short time?
An Australian giant cuttlefish: ‘It is no stretch to say they have personalities.’
Photograph: Peter Godfrey-Smith
Inches
above the seafloor of Sydney’s Cabbage Tree Bay, with the proximity
made possible by several millimetres of neoprene and an oxygen tank, I’m
just about eyeball to eyeball with this creature: an Australian giant
cuttlefish.
Even allowing for the magnifying effects of the mask snug across my
nose, it must be about two feet long, and the peculiarities that abound
in the cephalopod family, that includes octopuses and squid, are the more striking writ so large.
Its body – shaped around an internal surfboard-like shell, tailing
off into a fistful of tentacles – has the shifting colour of velvet in
light, and its W-shaped pupils lend it a stern expression. I don’t think
I’m imagining some recognition on its part. The question is, of what?
It was an encounter like this one – “at exactly the same place, actually, to the foot” – that first prompted Peter Godfrey-Smith
to think about these most other of minds. An Australian academic
philosopher, he’d recently been appointed a professor at Harvard.
While snorkelling on a visit home to Sydney in about 2007, he came
across a giant cuttlefish. The experience had a profound effect on him,
establishing an unlikely framework for his own study of philosophy,
first at Harvard and then the City University of New York.
The cuttlefish hadn’t been afraid – it had seemed as curious about
him as he was about it. But to imagine cephalopods’ experience of the
world as some iteration of our own may sell them short, given the many
millions of years of separation between us – nearly twice as many as
with humans and any other vertebrate (mammal, bird or fish).
Elle Hunt with an Australian giant cuttlefish at Cabbage Tree Bay, Manly, Sydney. Photograph: Peter Godfrey-Smith
Cephalopods’ high-resolution camera eyes resemble our own, but we
otherwise differ in every way. Octopuses in particular are peculiarly
other. The majority of their 500m neurons are in their arms, which can
not only touch but smell and taste – they quite literally have minds of
their own.
That it was possible to observe some kind of subjective experience, a
sense of self, in cephalopods fascinated Godfrey-Smith. How that might
differ to humans’ is the subject of his book Other Minds: The Octopus, The Sea and the Deep Origins of Consciousness, published this month by HarperCollins.
In it Godfrey-Smith charts his path through philosophical problems as
guided by cephalopods – in one case quite literally, when he recounts
an octopus taking his collaborator by hand on a 10-minute tour to its
den, “as if he were being led across the sea floor by a very small
eight-legged child”.
Charming anecdotes like this abound in Godfrey-Smith’s book,
particularly about captive octopuses frustrating scientists’ attempts at
observation.
A 1959 paper detailed an attempt at the Naples Zoological Station to
teach three octopuses to pull and release a lever in exchange for food.
Albert and Bertram performed in a “reasonably consistent” manner, but
one named Charles tried to drag a light suspended above the water into
the tank; squirted water at anyone who approached; and prematurely ended
the experiment when he broke the lever.
Most aquariums that have attempted to keep octopuses have tales to
tell of their great escapes – even their overnight raids of neighbouring
tanks for food. Godfrey-Smith writes of animals learning to turn off
lights by directing jets of water at them, short-circuiting the power
supply. Elsewhere octopuses have plugged their tanks’ outflow valves,
causing them to overflow.
This apparent problem-solving ability has led cephalopods
(particularly octopuses, because they’ve been studied more than squid or
cuttlefish) to be recognised as intelligent. Half a billion neurons put
octopuses close to the range of dogs and their brains are large
relative to their size, both of which offer biologists a rough guide to
brainpower.
The coconut octopus is one of the few cephalopods known to exhibit the behaviour of using a tool. Photograph: Mike Veitch/Alamy
In captivity, they have learned to navigate simple mazes, solve
puzzles and open screw-top jars, while wild animals have been observed
stacking rocks to protect the entrances to their dens, and hiding
themselves inside coconut shell halves.
But that’s also reflective of their dexterity: an animal with fewer
than eight legs may accomplish less but not necessarily because it is
more stupid. There’s no one metric by which to measure intelligence –
some markers, such as tool use, were settled on simply because they were
evident in humans.
“I
think it’s a mistake to look for a single, definitive thing,” says
Godfrey-Smith. “Octopuses are pretty good at sophisticated kinds of
learning, but how good it’s hard to say, in part because they’re so hard
to experiment on. You get a small amount of animals in the lab and some
of them refuse to do anything you want them to do – they’re just too
unruly.”
He sees that curiosity and opportunism – their “mischief and craft”,
as a Roman natural historian put it in the third century AD – as
characteristic of octopus intelligence.
Their great escapes from captivity, too, reflect an awareness of
their special circumstances and their ability to adapt to them. A 2010
experiment confirmed anecdotal reports that cephalopods are able to
recognise – and like or dislike – individual humans, even those that are
dressed identically.
It is no stretch to say they have personalities. But the
inconsistencies of their behaviour, combined with their apparent
intelligence, presents an obvious trap of anthropomorphism. It’s
“tempting”, admits Godfrey-Smith, to attribute their many enigmas to
“some clever, human-like explanation”.
A paradox: octopuses have big brains and short life spans. Photograph: Peter Godfrey-Smith
Opinions of octopus intelligence consequently vary within the
scientific community. A fundamental precept of animal psychology, coined
by the 19th-century British psychologist C Lloyd Morgan, says no
behaviour should be attributed to a sophisticated internal process if it
can be explained by a simpler one.
That is indicative of a general preference for simplicity of
hypotheses in science, says Godfrey-Smith, that as a philosopher he is
not convinced by. But scientific research across the board has become
more outcome-driven as a result of the cycle of funding and publishing,
and he is in the privileged position of being able to ask open-ended
questions.
“That’s a great luxury, to be able to roam around year after year, putting pieces together very slowly.”
That process, set in motion by his chance encounter with a cuttlefish
a decade ago, is ongoing. Now back based in Australia, lecturing at the
University of Sydney, Godfrey-Smith says his study of cephalopods is
increasingly influencing his professional life (and his personal one: Arrival,
the 2016 film about first contact with “cephalopod-esque” aliens, was a
“good, inventive film”, he says, though the invaders “were a bit more
like jellyfish”).
When philosophers ponder the mind-body problem, none poses quite such
a challenge as that of the octopus’s, and the study of cephalopods
gives some clues to questions about the origins of our own
consciousness.
Our last common ancestor existed 600m years ago and was thought to
resemble a flattened worm, perhaps only millimetres long. Yet somewhere
along the line, cephalopods developed high-resolution, camera eyes – as
did we, entirely independently.
“A camera eye, with a lens that focuses an image on a retina – we’ve
got it, they’ve got it, and that’s it,” says Godfrey-Smith. That it was
“arrived at twice” in such vastly different animals gives pause for
thought about the process of evolution, as does their inexplicably short
life spans: many species of cephalopods live only about three to five
years.
The study of cephalopods gives some clues to questions
about the origins of our own consciousness. Photograph: Peter
Godfrey-Smith
“When I learned that, I was just amazed – it was such a surprise,”
says Godfrey-Smith, somewhat sadly. “I’d just gotten to know the
animals. I thought, ‘I’ll be visiting these guys for ages.’ Then I
thought, ‘No, I won’t, they’ll be dead in a few months.’”
It’s
perhaps the biggest paradox presented by an animal that has no shortage
of contradictions: “A really big brain and a really short life.” From
an evolutionary perspective, Godfrey-Smith explains, it does not give a
good return on investment.
“It’s a bit like spending a vast amount of money to do a PhD, and
then you’ve got two years to make use of it ... the accounting is really
weird.”
One possibility is that an octopus’s brain needs to be powerful just
to preside over such an unwieldy form, in the same way that a computer
would need a state-of-the-art processor to perform a large volume of
complex tasks.
“I mean, the body is so hard to control, with eight arms and every
possible inch an elbow.” But that explanation doesn’t account for the
flair, even playfulness with which they apply it.
“They behave smartly, they do all these novel, inventive things –
that line of reasoning doesn’t resolve things, by any stretch,” says
Godfrey-Smith. “There’s still a somewhat mysterious element there.”
Other Minds: The Octopus and the Evolution of Intelligent Life
is published by William Collins. To order a copy for £17 (RRP £20) go to bookshop.theguardian.com or call 0330 333 6846. Free UK p&p over £10, online orders only. Phone orders min p&p of £1.99.
No comments:
Post a Comment